Psychology / en 鶹Ƶ study challenges stereotypes about lazy, unmotivated cannabis users /news/u-t-study-challenges-stereotypes-about-lazy-unmotivated-cannabis-users <span class="field field--name-title field--type-string field--label-hidden">鶹Ƶ study challenges stereotypes about lazy, unmotivated cannabis users</span> <div class="field field--name-field-featured-picture field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="eager" srcset="/sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_370/public/2024-04/Cannabis-web-lead.jpg?h=81d682ee&amp;itok=-_6vi8yE 370w, /sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_740/public/2024-04/Cannabis-web-lead.jpg?h=81d682ee&amp;itok=zyNqV28Y 740w, /sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_1110/public/2024-04/Cannabis-web-lead.jpg?h=81d682ee&amp;itok=tXf5kb6O 1110w" sizes="(min-width:1200px) 1110px, (max-width: 1199px) 80vw, (max-width: 767px) 90vw, (max-width: 575px) 95vw" width="740" height="494" src="/sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_370/public/2024-04/Cannabis-web-lead.jpg?h=81d682ee&amp;itok=-_6vi8yE" alt="A row of marijuana plants being grown in a commercial greenhouse"> </div> <span class="field field--name-uid field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden"><span>rahul.kalvapalle</span></span> <span class="field field--name-created field--type-created field--label-hidden"><time datetime="2024-04-29T13:51:28-04:00" title="Monday, April 29, 2024 - 13:51" class="datetime">Mon, 04/29/2024 - 13:51</time> </span> <div class="clearfix text-formatted field field--name-field-cutline-long field--type-text-long field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Cutline</div> <div class="field__item"><p><em>(Bloomberg Creative Photos via Getty Images)</em></p> </div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-author-reporters field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden field__items"> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/authors-reporters/don-campbell" hreflang="en">Don Campbell</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-topic field--type-entity-reference field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Topic</div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/topics/breaking-research" hreflang="en">Breaking Research</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-story-tags field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden field__items"> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/cannabis" hreflang="en">Cannabis</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/psychology" hreflang="en">Psychology</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/rotman-school-management" hreflang="en">Rotman School of Management</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/u-t-scarborough" hreflang="en">鶹Ƶ Scarborough</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-subheadline field--type-string-long field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Subheadline</div> <div class="field__item">"Our data suggests that you can be hard-working, motivated and a chronic cannabis user at the same time."<br> </div> </div> <div class="clearfix text-formatted field field--name-body field--type-text-with-summary field--label-hidden field__item"><p>Cannabis users might not be as lazy and unmotivated as popular stereotypes suggest, according to new research from the University of Toronto.</p> <p>In a study <a href="https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/19485506241245744">published in the journal <em>Social Psychological and Personality Science</em></a>, researchers found that regular cannabis use had minimal effects on motivation and willpower, and that getting high was associated with more positive emotions and fewer negative ones.</p> <p>The research aimed to take an objective look at the effects of recreational cannabis on the daily lives of chronic users, says <strong>Michael Inzlicht</strong>, a professor in the department of psychology at 鶹Ƶ Scarborough who led the study.&nbsp;</p> <p>“There is a stereotype that chronic cannabis users are somehow lazy or unproductive,” says Inzlicht, who is cross-appointed to the Rotman School of Management. “We found that’s not the case – their behaviours might change a bit in the moment while they’re high, but our evidence shows they are not lazy or lacking motivation at all.”&nbsp;</p> <figure role="group" class="caption caption-drupal-media align-left"> <div> <div class="field field--name-field-media-image field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="lazy" src="/sites/default/files/2024-04/embed_Inzlicht.jpg" width="300" height="450" alt="&quot;&quot;"> </div> </div> <figcaption><em>Professor Michael Inzlicht runs the Work and Play Lab at 鶹Ƶ Scarborough (photo by Lorne Bridgeman)</em></figcaption> </figure> <p>For the study, scientists surveyed 260 chronic cannabis users – defined as those who used cannabis at least three times a week – five times per day over the course of a week. Participants received notifications through an app asking if they were high, and were then prompted to answer questionnaires that assessed their emotional state, motivation levels, willpower and self-regulation.</p> <p>Inzlicht says the most interesting finding relates to motivation, with participants found to be just as willing and motivated to exert effort in completing a task when high compared to when sober.</p> <p>Past research has shown mixed results when it comes to chronic cannabis use and motivation, with Inzlicht noting much of it relied on limited experimental designs that didn't account for differences between cannabis users and non-users, including variations in personality, mental health or use of other psychoactive substances.</p> <p>He says this study accounted for those pre-existing differences and also looked at chronic cannabis use while participants were actively high.</p> <p>The researchers did find that being high was associated with lower levels of self-regulation, an important trait for being able to accomplish tasks. Specifically, chronic users reported being more impulsive, less thoughtful and less orderly.</p> <p>“These things can detract someone from getting stuff done, but we didn’t find it made them less hard-working, responsible or able to focus,” says Inzlicht, who runs the&nbsp;Work and Play Lab, which does research on self-control, motivation and empathy as well as social media, digital devices and recreational cannabis use.&nbsp;</p> <p>Chronic cannabis users were also found to experience a boost in positive emotions such as awe and gratitude when they were high, and a reduction in some negative emotions such as fear and anxiety. However, more chronic users were found to experience more negative emotions while high as well as while sober.</p> <p>The study also found no evidence of a “weed hangover” the day after cannabis use.</p> <p>Inzlicht notes that studying the effects of daily cannabis use was difficult in the past due to its legal status, and that most scholarship on the topic focused on negative impacts in an effort to curb use.</p> <p>Now that cannabis is legal in Canada, however, Inzlicht says he expects there will be more research focusing on both positive effects and risks.</p> <p>“The cannabis literature, historically, tended to focus a lot on the negative medical consequences of chronic use,” says Inzlicht. “Part of the motivation for this study is to take a neutral, clear-eyed approach to see how cannabis affects chronic users in their everyday lives.”&nbsp;</p> <p>He adds the study isn’t an endorsement of heavy cannabis use, pointing out there is plenty of research highlighting its risks – especially among adolescents.&nbsp;</p> <p>Rather, he points to Statistics Canada data showing that nearly one in 10 adult Canadians are regular cannabis users, and they come from all walks of life. Cannabis is also the fourth most used recreational drug after caffeine, alcohol and tobacco. But despite its increased legal and social acceptance, relatively little is known about the everyday experiences of regular users. &nbsp;</p> <p>“Our data suggests that you can be hard-working, motivated and a chronic cannabis user at the same time.”&nbsp;</p> <p>The study received funding from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada.</p> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-news-home-page-banner field--type-boolean field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">News home page banner</div> <div class="field__item">Off</div> </div> Mon, 29 Apr 2024 17:51:28 +0000 rahul.kalvapalle 307652 at Defy Dementia podcast inspires people of all ages to improve brain health … including its expert co-host /news/defy-dementia-podcast-inspires-people-all-ages-improve-brain-health-including-its-expert-co <span class="field field--name-title field--type-string field--label-hidden">Defy Dementia&nbsp;podcast inspires&nbsp;people of all ages&nbsp;to improve&nbsp;brain health … including&nbsp;its expert co-host</span> <div class="field field--name-field-featured-picture field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="eager" srcset="/sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_370/public/2024-04/Allison-Sekuler---headshot-2-crop.jpg?h=81d682ee&amp;itok=Og32PuqL 370w, /sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_740/public/2024-04/Allison-Sekuler---headshot-2-crop.jpg?h=81d682ee&amp;itok=oPxg4zUI 740w, /sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_1110/public/2024-04/Allison-Sekuler---headshot-2-crop.jpg?h=81d682ee&amp;itok=T47fBtIp 1110w" sizes="(min-width:1200px) 1110px, (max-width: 1199px) 80vw, (max-width: 767px) 90vw, (max-width: 575px) 95vw" width="740" height="494" src="/sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_370/public/2024-04/Allison-Sekuler---headshot-2-crop.jpg?h=81d682ee&amp;itok=Og32PuqL" alt="&quot;&quot;"> </div> <span class="field field--name-uid field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden"><span>Christopher.Sorensen</span></span> <span class="field field--name-created field--type-created field--label-hidden"><time datetime="2024-04-17T13:04:48-04:00" title="Wednesday, April 17, 2024 - 13:04" class="datetime">Wed, 04/17/2024 - 13:04</time> </span> <div class="clearfix text-formatted field field--name-field-cutline-long field--type-text-long field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Cutline</div> <div class="field__item"><p><em>Allison Sekuler, a cognitive neuroscientist, </em>is among the researchers at the Baycrest Centre who are working to advance the field of “precision aging” by applying the principles of precision medicine to aging and brain health <em>(photo by&nbsp;Donna Waxman)</em></p> </div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-author-reporters field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden field__items"> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/authors-reporters/adina-bresge" hreflang="en">Adina Bresge</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-topic field--type-entity-reference field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Topic</div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/topics/our-community" hreflang="en">Our Community</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-story-tags field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden field__items"> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/baycrest" hreflang="en">Baycrest </a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/faculty-arts-science" hreflang="en">Faculty of Arts &amp; Science</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/psychology" hreflang="en">Psychology</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-subheadline field--type-string-long field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Subheadline</div> <div class="field__item">Nominated for a Webby award, Defy Dementia was created by Allison Sekuler, a researcher at the Baycrest Centre and 鶹Ƶ psychology professor, and science communicator Jay Ingram&nbsp;</div> </div> <div class="clearfix text-formatted field field--name-body field--type-text-with-summary field--label-hidden field__item"><p>Cognitive neuroscientist&nbsp;<strong>Allison&nbsp;Sekuler&nbsp;</strong>says she and science communicator&nbsp;<strong>Jay Ingram</strong>&nbsp;launched the&nbsp;<em><a href="https://www.baycrest.org/podcast/episode-11" target="_blank">Defy Dementia</a></em>&nbsp;podcast to empower people to make simple changes to improve their brain health.&nbsp;</p> <p>What she didn’t expect was that the project would inspire her to make changes of her own.</p> <p>As the Baycrest Centre’s Sandra&nbsp;A. Rotman Chair in cognitive neuroscience at the Rotman Research Institute and president and chief scientist of Baycrest Academy for Research and Education and Centre for Aging + Brain Health Innovation, Sekuler is well-versed in the growing body of science linking dementia risk to lifestyle factors such as social isolation, sleep and exercise.&nbsp;</p> <p>But when&nbsp;<em>Defy Dementia</em>&nbsp;launched a year ago, Sekuler admits that she had fallen into a rut on the fitness front. Despite continuing to pay monthly membership fees, she hadn’t&nbsp;set foot in a gym since the COVID-19 lockdowns in early 2020.</p> <p>That soon changed as she and Ingram heard from listeners who were putting the podcast’s advice into practice.</p> <p>“It’s been gratifying, intellectually, that we're able to make a difference in people's lives,” says Sekuler, who is a professor of psychology in the University of Toronto’s Faculty of Arts &amp; Science. “But it's also had a deep personal impact on me, inspiring me to get myself up off the couch and start becoming more active.”</p> <p>In particular, she credits two podcast conversations with getting her back into the gym: a pep talk from&nbsp;<a href="https://ernestineshepherd.net/?page_id=2" target="_blank">octogenarian bodybuilder&nbsp;<strong>Ernestine Shepherd</strong></a>, and the story of listener-turned-guest&nbsp;<strong>Ravi Venkatesh</strong>,&nbsp;an IT specialist in Mississauga, Ont., who told the hosts how the podcast had motivated him to recommit to his yoga regimen.</p> <p>As Sekuler sees it, she’s just one beneficiary of the community of learning that has sprung up around&nbsp;<em>Defy Dementia</em>, which is currently vying for a voter-chosen&nbsp;Webby Award for podcasts in the health, wellness and lifestyle category&nbsp;(<a href="https://vote.webbyawards.com/PublicVoting#/2024/podcasts/shows/health-wellness-lifestyle" target="_blank">voting is open until April 18</a>).</p> <p><iframe class="podcastdotco-player podcastdotco-player--episode" data-gtm-yt-inspected-11="true" data-gtm-yt-inspected-1298258_37="true" data-target="defy-dementia/dd-ep-11" frameborder="0" scrolling="no" src="https://play.pod.co/defy-dementia/ep-11" style="overflow:hidden;max-width:750px;height:160px;" width="100%"></iframe></p> <p>A general term encompassing several diseases that impair memory, thinking and reasoning, dementia is projected to afflict 150 million people by 2050, including more than one million in Canada, according to&nbsp;<a href="https://www.thelancet.com/journals/lanpub/article/PIIS2468-2667(21)00249-8/fulltext" target="_blank">research published in&nbsp;<em>The Lancet</em></a>.</p> <p>“Obviously, nobody wants to get dementia, and there are things you can do to decrease your risk. But even people who are living with the disease, it's not necessarily, you know, a death sentence,” says Sekuler, whose research specializes in the links between perception, memory and aging.</p> <p>“Our goal was … to portray a realistic view of the disease and of aging, but [also] to show the possibilities that are there.”</p> <p>Sekuler is among the scientists at the Baycrest Centre working to advance the field of “precision aging,” applying the principles of precision medicine to aging and brain health to detect, treat and care for people with dementia in a personalized way.</p> <p>Research suggests we have more control over dementia than one might think – <a href="https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7392084/" target="_blank">a&nbsp;2020 report&nbsp;by the&nbsp;<em>Lancet</em> Commission</a> found a dozen modifiable risk factors account for 40 per cent of dementias, including hearing impairment, smoking and obesity – but&nbsp;Sekuler stresses that many of the interventions under development are only effective if the disease is caught early.</p> <p>Hence, a key challenge facing the field is the popular belief that dementia is a disease determined solely by your genes.</p> <p>On&nbsp;<em>Defy Dementia</em>, Sekuler and Ingram, who have been collaborating since the 1990s, push back against this misconception by offering tips on reducing risk through lifestyle changes. That includes episodes focused on subjects such as cognitive engagement, nutrition, sleep and stress.</p> <p>Sekuler attributes much of the podcast’s resonance to its format. Each episode features one guest sharing their personal experience with dementia, followed by a scientist who responds to the story with expert context and insights.</p> <p>“It made the science that much more accessible and relatable,” she says.</p> <p>Sekuler says she was surprised to find that people of all ages are tuning in – not just the older crowd that she and Ingram initially expected to attract.&nbsp;</p> <p>“You’re never too young or too old to take care of your brain,” Sekuler says. “The sooner you start taking actions to improve your lifestyle, the more it becomes a habit for you throughout your life.”</p> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-news-home-page-banner field--type-boolean field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">News home page banner</div> <div class="field__item">Off</div> </div> Wed, 17 Apr 2024 17:04:48 +0000 Christopher.Sorensen 307503 at Personality type can help predict who's single or in a relationship - and how happy they are: Study /news/personality-type-can-help-predict-who-s-single-or-relationship-and-how-happy-they-are-study <span class="field field--name-title field--type-string field--label-hidden">Personality type can help predict who's single or in a relationship - and how happy they are: Study</span> <div class="field field--name-field-featured-picture field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="eager" srcset="/sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_370/public/2024-04/iStock-1322920245-crop.jpg?h=7c2480be&amp;itok=mHMmkr2K 370w, /sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_740/public/2024-04/iStock-1322920245-crop.jpg?h=7c2480be&amp;itok=bwafCXV8 740w, /sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_1110/public/2024-04/iStock-1322920245-crop.jpg?h=7c2480be&amp;itok=d3ZD2e9v 1110w" sizes="(min-width:1200px) 1110px, (max-width: 1199px) 80vw, (max-width: 767px) 90vw, (max-width: 575px) 95vw" width="740" height="494" src="/sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_370/public/2024-04/iStock-1322920245-crop.jpg?h=7c2480be&amp;itok=mHMmkr2K" alt="man relaxing on a sofa at home"> </div> <span class="field field--name-uid field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden"><span>Christopher.Sorensen</span></span> <span class="field field--name-created field--type-created field--label-hidden"><time datetime="2024-04-10T13:20:47-04:00" title="Wednesday, April 10, 2024 - 13:20" class="datetime">Wed, 04/10/2024 - 13:20</time> </span> <div class="clearfix text-formatted field field--name-field-cutline-long field--type-text-long field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Cutline</div> <div class="field__item"><p><em>(photo by Prostock Studio/Getty Images)</em></p> </div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-author-reporters field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden field__items"> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/authors-reporters/josslyn-johnstone" hreflang="en">Josslyn Johnstone</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-topic field--type-entity-reference field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Topic</div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/topics/breaking-research" hreflang="en">Breaking Research</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-story-tags field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden field__items"> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/faculty-arts-science" hreflang="en">Faculty of Arts &amp; Science</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/graduate-students" hreflang="en">Graduate Students</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/psychology" hreflang="en">Psychology</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/research-innovation" hreflang="en">Research &amp; Innovation</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-subheadline field--type-string-long field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Subheadline</div> <div class="field__item">"Our study contributes to a more complex picture of single lives that goes beyond the misleading stereotype of the miserable single person”</div> </div> <div class="clearfix text-formatted field field--name-body field--type-text-with-summary field--label-hidden field__item"><p>The phrase “single life” may conjure images of a busy&nbsp;<em>Sex and the City</em>-like social calendar, packed with dates and drama. But researchers at the University of Toronto say most singles are actually introverts – a far cry from the extroverted stereotypes&nbsp;we often see depicted in movies and on TV.</p> <p>In a study&nbsp;<a href="https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/01461672231225571" target="_blank">published recently in the&nbsp;</a><em><a href="https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/01461672231225571" target="_blank">Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin</a>,</em>&nbsp;the researchers reveal how certain personality traits – particularly how extroverted, conscientious and neurotic someone is – predict who is likelier to be single or in a relationship.&nbsp;</p> <p>It’s one of several links between personality, well-being and relationship status described in the research.</p> <figure role="group" class="caption caption-drupal-media align-left"> <div> <div class="field field--name-field-media-image field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="lazy" src="/sites/default/files/2024-04/elaine-hoan-portrait-crop.jpg" width="300" height="352" alt="&quot;&quot;"> </div> </div> <figcaption><em>Elaine Hoan (supplied image)</em></figcaption> </figure> <p>“As marriage rates decline and more people live alone, our study contributes to a more complex picture of single lives that goes beyond the misleading stereotype of the miserable single person,” says lead author&nbsp;<strong>Elaine Hoan</strong>, a PhD candidate in in the lab of <strong>Geoff&nbsp;MacDonald</strong>, a professor in the department of psychology in the Faculty of Arts &amp; Science.</p> <p>“While on average people in relationships are more satisfied with their lives than single people, there are many happy singles – relationships don't play as big of a role in one’s overall life satisfaction as you may think.</p> <p>“We found that personality, more than relationship status, determines who is happy with their life and who isn’t.”</p> <p>For the study, researchers recruited over 1,800 participants between the ages of 20 and 59 who had either been single for at least six months or in a relationship for at least six months. Participants completed a set of questionnaires that measured personality, satisfaction with relationship status, sexual satisfaction and life satisfaction to shed light on how personality traits affect well-being in the context of relationship status.</p> <p>For the personality measurement, Hoan used the “Big Five” model of personality, which focuses on the following traits: extroversion (outgoing and high-energy), agreeableness (compassionate and respectful), conscientiousness (productive and dependable), neuroticism (anxious and depressed) and openness (curious and creative).</p> <p>Where someone fell on the introversion and extraversion scale was more strongly related to whether they would be single or in a relationship.</p> <p>“In a world that caters to extroverts, introverts are misrepresented as antisocial,” says Hoan. “The reality is, introverts enjoy their alone time and independence, and can emotionally regulate – meaning, they can manage their reactions to their feelings on their own. So, an introvert may prefer being single more than being in a relationship.</p> <p>“On the other hand, extroverts are happier than introverts in general – regardless of whether they were single or not – and introversion makes it a bit tougher to get into a relationship in the first place because introverts may not find themselves in social situations as often.</p> <p>“Other existing research also suggests that being in a relationship may make someone more extroverted, by increasing confidence and widening their social circle.”</p> <p>The researchers also found notable, though weaker, connections to the traits of conscientiousness and neuroticism. Single people were less likely to identify with descriptors like “keeps things neat and tidy” and “gets things done” and agreed more strongly with phrases like “can be tense”, “often feels sad” and “is temperamental.”</p> <p>“Conscientious people are more likely to be goal-oriented, especially towards traditional goals like getting a job and getting married, as well as exhibit a strong work ethic, so that may feed into their desire and ability to start and commit to a romantic relationship,” says Hoan. “Depressive symptoms like sadness and low energy may make it more difficult to pursue and maintain a relationship – while the emotional support you get in a romantic relationship could reduce these symptoms.”</p> <p>Next, Hoan is researching happiness in married versus unmarried people. She hopes her work continues to challenge societal misconceptions about who people are and how they choose to live their lives, whether in a relationship or not.</p> <p>“There is stigma associated with being single – you know, people saying, ‘You’d be happier if you were in a relationship, so why aren’t you?’ – when that’s not necessarily true for everyone,”&nbsp;she says.&nbsp;</p> <p>“I hope what people take from our research is the idea that you don’t have to be someone you’re not – just be yourself.”</p> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-news-home-page-banner field--type-boolean field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">News home page banner</div> <div class="field__item">Off</div> </div> Wed, 10 Apr 2024 17:20:47 +0000 Christopher.Sorensen 307363 at Little evidence linking five 'love languages' to healthy relationships, researchers say /news/little-evidence-linking-five-love-languages-healthy-relationships-researchers-say <span class="field field--name-title field--type-string field--label-hidden">Little evidence linking five 'love languages' to healthy relationships, researchers say</span> <div class="field field--name-field-featured-picture field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="eager" srcset="/sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_370/public/2024-01/pexels-shvets-production-8933541-crop.jpg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=9Q7kv6Z5 370w, /sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_740/public/2024-01/pexels-shvets-production-8933541-crop.jpg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=_QSa4_xS 740w, /sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_1110/public/2024-01/pexels-shvets-production-8933541-crop.jpg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=4BnFE4NE 1110w" sizes="(min-width:1200px) 1110px, (max-width: 1199px) 80vw, (max-width: 767px) 90vw, (max-width: 575px) 95vw" width="740" height="494" src="/sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_370/public/2024-01/pexels-shvets-production-8933541-crop.jpg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=9Q7kv6Z5" alt="A man opens a gift received from his partner"> </div> <span class="field field--name-uid field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden"><span>Christopher.Sorensen</span></span> <span class="field field--name-created field--type-created field--label-hidden"><time datetime="2024-01-04T10:16:15-05:00" title="Thursday, January 4, 2024 - 10:16" class="datetime">Thu, 01/04/2024 - 10:16</time> </span> <div class="clearfix text-formatted field field--name-field-cutline-long field--type-text-long field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Cutline</div> <div class="field__item"><p><em>Assumptions around love languages, such as physical touch and gifts, don't hold up to scientific scrutiny, according to research by&nbsp;鶹Ƶ Mississauga psychologist Emily Impett and her research partners (photo via Pexels)</em></p> </div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-author-reporters field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden field__items"> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/authors-reporters/megan-easton" hreflang="en">Megan Easton</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-topic field--type-entity-reference field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Topic</div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/topics/breaking-research" hreflang="en">Breaking Research</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-story-tags field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden field__items"> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/graduate-students" hreflang="en">Graduate Students</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/psychology" hreflang="en">Psychology</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/relationships" hreflang="en">Relationships</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/research-innovation" hreflang="en">Research &amp; Innovation</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/u-t-mississauga" hreflang="en">鶹Ƶ Mississauga</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-subheadline field--type-string-long field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Subheadline</div> <div class="field__item">鶹Ƶ Mississauga's Emily Impett and her collaborators say good relationships are more like a balanced diet, where people receive a wide range of essential nutrients </div> </div> <div class="clearfix text-formatted field field--name-body field--type-text-with-summary field--label-hidden field__item"><p>Even if you don’t know your love language, you’ve probably heard of the concept.</p> <p>The theory’s pervasiveness in pop culture has only increased in the 30-odd years since Baptist minister Gary Chapman published his book&nbsp;<em>The Five Love Languages: The Secret to Love That Lasts</em>.&nbsp;&nbsp;</p> <p>But when psychology researchers at the University of Toronto decided to test Chapman’s main assumptions, they found they don’t stand up to scientific scrutiny.&nbsp;</p> <figure role="group" class="caption caption-drupal-media align-left"> <div> <div class="field field--name-field-media-image field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="lazy" src="/sites/default/files/2024-01/Emily-Impett-supplied-image.jpg" width="300" height="400" alt="&quot;&quot;"> </div> </div> <figcaption><em>Emily Impett (supplied image)</em></figcaption> </figure> <p>“We were very skeptical about the love languages idea, so we decided to review the existing studies on it,” says&nbsp;<strong>Emily Impett</strong>, a professor in 鶹Ƶ Mississauga’s department of psychology who collaborated with graduate student&nbsp;<strong>Gideon Park</strong>&nbsp;and York University Assistant Professor <strong>Amy Muise</strong>.</p> <p>“None of the 10 studies supported Chapman’s claims.”</p> <p>For example, Chapman uses the language metaphor to represent how individuals tend to prefer giving and receiving love. The notion rests on three premises: that every person has a primary love language, that there are five love languages (physical touch, words of affirmation, acts of service, quality time and gifts), and that when couples “speak” the same love language it improves the quality of their relationships.&nbsp;</p> <p>But each of these assertions broke down when Impett and her team evaluated them against the 10 studies they reviewed (the team’s results are scheduled to be published in the journal&nbsp;<em>Current Directions in Psychological Science)</em>.&nbsp;</p> <p>“People determine their primary love language by taking Chapman’s quiz, which forces them to select the expressions of love they find most meaningful,” says Impett, who is also the director of <a href="https://www.emilyimpett.com/">the&nbsp;Relationships and Well-Being Laboratory</a>. “It could be choosing between receiving gifts or holding hands, for example. These are trade-offs we don’t have to make in real life.</p> <p>“In fact, people report that they find all of the things described by the love languages to be incredibly important in a relationship.”&nbsp;&nbsp;</p> <p>When it comes to the number of love languages, the studies found inconsistent evidence for the five languages Chapman identifies, while other relationship research shows there are additional ways of expressing and receiving love.</p> <p>“One key thing to remember is that Chapman developed the five love languages by working with a sample of white, religious, mixed-gender, traditional couples,” says Impett. “There are certain things that are left out, such as affirming a partner’s personal goals outside of the relationship, which might be significant to couples with more egalitarian values.”&nbsp;</p> <p>Most importantly, Impett and her team found no scientific evidence for Chapman’s central contention that people who choose partners that speak their love language, or learn to speak it, will have more successful relationships.</p> <p>“There’s no support for this matching effect,” says Impett. “People are basically happier in relationships when they receive any of these expressions of love.”&nbsp;&nbsp;</p> <p>Impett and her collaborators recognize that people crave easy tools to enhance their love lives&nbsp;– which helps explain why Chapman’s book has sold millions of copies and turned the “love languages” into romantic shorthand.</p> <p>“Everyone wants to be in a good relationship, so we didn’t just say the love languages are scientifically debunked and stop there,” she says.</p> <p>The team offered an alternative metaphor&nbsp;– one that’s rooted in research.&nbsp;It proposes that relationships are a balanced diet, where people need a full range of essential nutrients (including the factors described by the five love languages and others such as companionship and emotional support) to nourish lasting love.</p> <p>“It keeps all expressions of love on the menu and invites partners to share what they need at different times,” says Impett. “It allows for the fact that people and relationships aren’t static and can’t be categorized into neat boxes.”&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;</p> <p>This is not the first time Impett has put&nbsp;common beliefs about relationships&nbsp;to the test. “I really like challenging these lay ideas because my goal is always to translate the best scientific evidence to therapists and the general public,” she says.&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;</p> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-news-home-page-banner field--type-boolean field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">News home page banner</div> <div class="field__item">Off</div> </div> Thu, 04 Jan 2024 15:16:15 +0000 Christopher.Sorensen 305138 at In his latest book, 鶹Ƶ prof tells the story of the human mind /news/his-latest-book-u-t-prof-tells-story-human-mind <span class="field field--name-title field--type-string field--label-hidden">In his latest book, 鶹Ƶ prof tells the story of the human mind</span> <div class="field field--name-field-featured-picture field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="eager" srcset="/sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_370/public/2023-11/paul-bloom-psych.jpg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=iQdWgukk 370w, /sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_740/public/2023-11/paul-bloom-psych.jpg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=0T2fZl-a 740w, /sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_1110/public/2023-11/paul-bloom-psych.jpg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=H5P17HLR 1110w" sizes="(min-width:1200px) 1110px, (max-width: 1199px) 80vw, (max-width: 767px) 90vw, (max-width: 575px) 95vw" width="740" height="494" src="/sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_370/public/2023-11/paul-bloom-psych.jpg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=iQdWgukk" alt="&quot;&quot;"> </div> <span class="field field--name-uid field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden"><span>Christopher.Sorensen</span></span> <span class="field field--name-created field--type-created field--label-hidden"><time datetime="2023-11-22T14:10:20-05:00" title="Wednesday, November 22, 2023 - 14:10" class="datetime">Wed, 11/22/2023 - 14:10</time> </span> <div class="clearfix text-formatted field field--name-field-cutline-long field--type-text-long field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Cutline</div> <div class="field__item"><p><em>Paul Bloom, a professor of psychology,&nbsp;provides an overview of major aspects of psychology in his seventh book:&nbsp;Psych: The Story of the Human Mind&nbsp;(photo by Greg Martin)</em></p> </div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-author-reporters field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden field__items"> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/authors-reporters/chris-sasaki" hreflang="en">Chris Sasaki</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-topic field--type-entity-reference field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Topic</div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/topics/our-community" hreflang="en">Our Community</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-story-tags field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden field__items"> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/faculty-arts-science" hreflang="en">Faculty of Arts &amp; Science</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/psychology" hreflang="en">Psychology</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-subheadline field--type-string-long field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Subheadline</div> <div class="field__item">Paul Bloom says psychology is "about the most interesting topic there is – us"</div> </div> <div class="clearfix text-formatted field field--name-body field--type-text-with-summary field--label-hidden field__item"><p><strong>Paul Bloom</strong>’s latest book kicks off by recounting a daydream in which he quits his job as a&nbsp;psychology professor to venture into the world of cosmology.&nbsp;&nbsp;</p> <p>But his fantasy of studying the vast reaches of the universe was brief because, he writes, “...all of psychology gives me this buzz. It’s about the most interesting topic there is – us. It’s about our feelings, experiences, plans, goals, fantasies, the most intimate aspects of our being.”</p> <p>A professor in the University of Toronto’s&nbsp;department of psychology in the Faculty of Arts &amp; Science, Bloom’s research focuses on developmental psychology, personality and how we make sense of the world&nbsp;– with a particular interest in pleasure, morality, religion, fiction and art.</p> <p>His seventh book, <em>Psych: The Story of the Human Mind</em>,&nbsp;had its origins in the introductory psychology course Bloom taught at Yale University and, as such, provides an excellent overview of major aspects of psychology.</p> <p>In its pages, he attempts to “put forth the best answers we have” to the fundamental questions most have pondered: How does the brain give rise to intelligence and conscious experience? Where does knowledge come from? How and why do we differ in personality, intelligence and other traits? How does the mind of a child differ from that of an adult? What makes people happy?</p> <p>He also provides an overview of the different schools of psychological thought that offered their own best answers to the queries.</p> <p>He describes the mechanistic or materialist point of view that our thoughts and emotions are the output of a vast assembly of nerve cells and the result of interactions between various molecules – or as Nobel Prize-winner and co-discoverer of the helical structure of DNA Francis Crick wrote: “Brain makes thought.”</p> <p>He also describes “Cartesian dualism,” named for French philosopher and scientist René Descartes – the idea that our minds and our physical selves are distinct entities. In other words, we are bodies&nbsp;and&nbsp;souls. We feel certain, Bloom writes, “we are not our bodies; we&nbsp;inhabit&nbsp;these bodies. We are&nbsp;Ghosts in the Shell.”</p> <p><em>Psych</em>&nbsp;is filled with anecdotes and intriguing facts that help explain psychological principles and bring them to life&nbsp;– insights that would’ve sparked curiosity and interest among the students who either enrolled in the Yale course or took the online version of it that Bloom later developed.</p> <p>He tells the story of Phineas Gage, who in 1848, while working on the construction of a railway, had a metal rod blasted through his brain. Gage survived and retained the ability to speak and understand language, as well as other intellectual capacities. However, Gage eventually suffered severe changes to his personality and emotional states, began having seizures and died – a clear demonstration of how physical damage to the brain affects who we are.</p> <p>Bloom wrote the book during COVID-19, and he uses the pandemic to discuss the question: “If we’re so smart, why do we often seem so dumb?” Why is Holocaust denial still rampant, why do some believe 9/11 was an “inside job,” why are there so many followers of QAnon who believe, among other absurdities, that the actor Tom Hanks is a Satanist?</p> <p>And why has COVID-19 become so politicized with so little regard to science? In writing about conspiracy theories like these, Bloom describes how beliefs about the disease contrast starkly between Democrats and Republicans (with neither side having a monopoly on accuracy). He cites a study showing that subjects supported a hypothetical government program, not on its merits, but on whether they were told it was a Democratic or Republican initiative.</p> <p>Nonetheless, Bloom is optimistic and writes that, despite our irrationality, “... we are also the animal that is capable of acting intelligently for the long-term good...and tries to see things as they really are.”</p> <p>The questions of consciousness and mind that Bloom raises in his book point to just how vast the story of the human mind is – despite its miniscule size compared to, say, a galaxy.</p> <p>“The more you look at the mind and how it works from a serious scientific point of view, the more you appreciate its complexity, its uniqueness, and its beauty.”</p> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-news-home-page-banner field--type-boolean field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">News home page banner</div> <div class="field__item">Off</div> </div> Wed, 22 Nov 2023 19:10:20 +0000 Christopher.Sorensen 304475 at Addicted to our devices: Researchers track global patterns of smartphone use /news/addicted-our-devices-researchers-track-global-patterns-smartphone-use <span class="field field--name-title field--type-string field--label-hidden">Addicted to our devices: Researchers track global patterns of smartphone use </span> <div class="field field--name-field-featured-picture field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="eager" srcset="/sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_370/public/2023-11/smartphone-pexels-eren-li-7241276-resized.jpg?h=1ec4aeb5&amp;itok=O4odokGO 370w, /sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_740/public/2023-11/smartphone-pexels-eren-li-7241276-resized.jpg?h=1ec4aeb5&amp;itok=ZdvAKwa3 740w, /sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_1110/public/2023-11/smartphone-pexels-eren-li-7241276-resized.jpg?h=1ec4aeb5&amp;itok=JpYp-u-a 1110w" sizes="(min-width:1200px) 1110px, (max-width: 1199px) 80vw, (max-width: 767px) 90vw, (max-width: 575px) 95vw" width="740" height="494" src="/sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_370/public/2023-11/smartphone-pexels-eren-li-7241276-resized.jpg?h=1ec4aeb5&amp;itok=O4odokGO" alt="young man lies in bed looking at smartphone"> </div> <span class="field field--name-uid field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden"><span>Christopher.Sorensen</span></span> <span class="field field--name-created field--type-created field--label-hidden"><time datetime="2023-11-08T09:58:05-05:00" title="Wednesday, November 8, 2023 - 09:58" class="datetime">Wed, 11/08/2023 - 09:58</time> </span> <div class="clearfix text-formatted field field--name-field-cutline-long field--type-text-long field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Cutline</div> <div class="field__item"><p><em>Researchers surveyed more than 50,000 participants across 195 countries in a bid to understand who is at risk of smartphone addiction (photo by Erin Li/Pexels)</em></p> </div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-author-reporters field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden field__items"> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/authors-reporters/ali-raza" hreflang="en">Ali Raza</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-topic field--type-entity-reference field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Topic</div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/topics/breaking-research" hreflang="en">Breaking Research</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-story-tags field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden field__items"> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/global" hreflang="en">Global</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/psychology" hreflang="en">Psychology</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/research-innovation" hreflang="en">Research &amp; Innovation</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/u-t-mississauga" hreflang="en">鶹Ƶ Mississauga</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/u-t-scarborough" hreflang="en">鶹Ƶ Scarborough</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-subheadline field--type-string-long field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Subheadline</div> <div class="field__item">Survey suggests likelihood of problematic smartphone use greater among women, young people</div> </div> <div class="clearfix text-formatted field field--name-body field--type-text-with-summary field--label-hidden field__item"><p>Are you addicted to your smartphone? If so, you're not alone.&nbsp;&nbsp;</p> <p>A team of researchers at the University of Toronto have collected the largest set of data in any study regarding problematic smartphone use.&nbsp;</p> <p>The study&nbsp;–&nbsp;<a href="https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s11469-023-01146-3">published in the <em>International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction</em></a>&nbsp;–&nbsp;found that among 41 countries with at least 100 participants, women scored higher than men in problematic smartphone use, with the results being inversely proportional to age. That is, the older a user is, the less likely they are to have problematic smartphone use.&nbsp;&nbsp;</p> <p>“We weren’t expecting this kind of robust consistency,” says study co-author&nbsp;<strong>Jay Olson</strong>, a post-doctoral researcher in the department of psychology at 鶹Ƶ Mississauga.</p> <figure role="group" class="caption caption-drupal-media align-right"> <div> <div class="field field--name-field-media-image field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="lazy" src="/sites/default/files/2023-11/Jay-Olson-Gabriel-Helfant-crop.jpg" width="250" height="251" alt="&quot;&quot;"> </div> </div> <figcaption><em>Jay Olson (photo by Gabriel Helfant)</em></figcaption> </figure> <p>The data also allowed researchers – including&nbsp;<strong>Dasha Sandra</strong>, a PhD student in the department of psychological clinical science at 鶹Ƶ Scarborough<strong>&nbsp;</strong>–&nbsp;to identify geographic patterns since it covered multiple countries across several continents.</p> <p>The highest scores of problematic use were found in Southeast Asia, while the lowest were in Europe.&nbsp;&nbsp;</p> <p>Olson says the results prompt “fundamental questions in the field” such as: Why are women shown to have higher rates of problematic smartphone use? What is it about young people that increases their likelihood of problematic use? And what are the social and cultural differences from country to country that influence these results?&nbsp;</p> <p>The researchers collected survey responses from 50,423 participants aged 18 to 90 across 195 countries. Survey participants answered questions according to the <a href="https://www.researchgate.net/figure/Smartphone-Addiction-Scale-Short-Version-SAS-SV_tbl1_351005231" target="_blank">Smartphone Addiction Scale</a>, a widely used measure to study problematic smartphone use.&nbsp;</p> <p>Olson says problematic use among women may have something to do with how each gender generally uses their smartphones.&nbsp;</p> <p>“Researchers think women tend to use their phones more for social reasons: communication with friends and family via social media,” he says, adding that uses related to social validation (for example, “likes” on Instagram) are “the kinds of uses that can build habits very quickly.”&nbsp;</p> <p>It’s not that men don’t use their phones for social reasons, he says, but it’s believed they tend to use fewer social functions such as keeping up with group chats, connecting with family or following influencers.&nbsp;&nbsp;</p> <p>The research team aims to test this hypothesis as they continue to analyze the data.&nbsp;&nbsp;</p> <p>Problematic usage can also reflect distress or anguish experienced by individuals.</p> <p>“People try to avoid negative emotions by using their phone – kind of like an adult pacifier,” he says, noting differences&nbsp;in both gender and age can be related to differences in distress levels.&nbsp;</p> <p>The research also accounts for cultural differences around smartphone use. Using an index of “cultural tightness” and “cultural looseness,” researchers examined the data through varying social norms across the globe. The distinction can also be made by examining collectivist cultures&nbsp;– which prioritize group connections&nbsp;– versus individualist cultures.&nbsp;&nbsp;</p> <p>Their hypothesis is that strictness of social norms plays a role in smartphone use. In an individualistic culture, for example, “it’s not expected that you would be calling your family every day,” Olson says.&nbsp;</p> <p>Another factor is&nbsp;the number of available screens competing for users’ attention. Olson notes that some countries bypassed widespread laptop and desktop computers in favour of developing mobile data networks, meaning that smartphones became people’s primary computing device.&nbsp;For example, Europe, which recorded the lowest rates of problematic use, has been using the internet since it first arrived on personal computers in the 1980s and 1990s. Southeast Asia, by contrast, recorded the highest rates of problematic use and had widespread adoption of the internet via smartphones in just the last 15 years.&nbsp;&nbsp;</p> <p>“It explains some differences in screen time,” Olson says.&nbsp;</p> <p>Not all smartphone use is problematic since it largely depends on how an individual is using a phone.&nbsp;</p> <p>“A social media manager could be logging eight hours of screen time a day, but this doesn’t necessarily have a problematic effect on your life versus somebody who uses their phone for half an hour from midnight to 12:30 a.m. while trying to fall asleep,” Olson explains.&nbsp;</p> <p>The research team plans to run a longer-term version of the survey&nbsp;to track smartphone use over time. It’s also looking into developing a habit-based intervention for problematic smartphone usage, for which data like this is critical.&nbsp;</p> <p>“This is in some sense a global experiment,” Olson says. “Smartphones became popular around 2008 and we’re just tracking the effects of this globally, post-hoc.”&nbsp;</p> <p>&nbsp;</p> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-news-home-page-banner field--type-boolean field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">News home page banner</div> <div class="field__item">Off</div> </div> Wed, 08 Nov 2023 14:58:05 +0000 Christopher.Sorensen 304342 at Infants prefer live music over recorded version, study finds /news/infants-prefer-live-music-over-recorded-version-study-finds <span class="field field--name-title field--type-string field--label-hidden">Infants prefer live music over recorded version, study finds</span> <div class="field field--name-field-featured-picture field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="eager" srcset="/sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_370/public/2023-07/babybanner-crop.jpg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=zWUioZIk 370w, /sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_740/public/2023-07/babybanner-crop.jpg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=CE75eCQX 740w, /sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_1110/public/2023-07/babybanner-crop.jpg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=DluGZ6yB 1110w" sizes="(min-width:1200px) 1110px, (max-width: 1199px) 80vw, (max-width: 767px) 90vw, (max-width: 575px) 95vw" width="740" height="494" src="/sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_370/public/2023-07/babybanner-crop.jpg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=zWUioZIk" alt="a delighted baby sitting outside"> </div> <span class="field field--name-uid field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden"><span>lanthierj</span></span> <span class="field field--name-created field--type-created field--label-hidden"><time datetime="2023-07-17T15:22:00-04:00" title="Monday, July 17, 2023 - 15:22" class="datetime">Mon, 07/17/2023 - 15:22</time> </span> <div class="clearfix text-formatted field field--name-field-cutline-long field--type-text-long field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Cutline</div> <div class="field__item"><p><em>Research found that babies' heart rates synchronized and they were more engaged when watching live music, compared to a recording of the same performance (photo by Envato Elements)</em></p> </div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-author-reporters field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden field__items"> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/authors-reporters/alexa-battler" hreflang="en">Alexa Battler</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-topic field--type-entity-reference field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Topic</div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/topics/breaking-research" hreflang="en">Breaking Research</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-story-tags field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden field__items"> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/babies" hreflang="en">Babies</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/child-development" hreflang="en">Child Development</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/connaught-fund" hreflang="en">Connaught Fund</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/music" hreflang="en">Music</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/nserc" hreflang="en">NSERC</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/psychology" hreflang="en">Psychology</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/research-innovation" hreflang="en">Research &amp; Innovation</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/university-toronto-scarborough" hreflang="en">University of Toronto Scarborough</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-subheadline field--type-string-long field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Subheadline</div> <div class="field__item">New research from 鶹Ƶ's ​TEMPO Lab suggests that even babies feel the impact of being at a live show, through both musicians’ interactions with an audience and the social experience of being in a crowd</div> </div> <div class="clearfix text-formatted field field--name-body field--type-text-with-summary field--label-hidden field__item"><p>You don't have to be an adult to feel the power of live opera&nbsp;– even babies prefer to attend in person, a new study suggests.</p> <p>When infants watched a live performance of a baby opera, their heart rates synchronized and they were significantly more engaged than babies who watched an identical recording of the show, researchers say.</p> <figure role="group" class="caption caption-drupal-media align-left"> <div> <div class="field field--name-field-media-image field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="lazy" src="/sites/default/files/2023-07/2023_Headshot%5B53-crop%5D.jpg" width="250" height="301" alt="&quot;&quot;"> </div> </div> <figcaption><em>Laura Cirelli (supplied image)</em></figcaption> </figure> <p>“Their heart rates were speeding up and slowing down in a similar fashion to other babies watching the show,” says <strong>Laura Cirelli</strong>, assistant professor in the department of psychology at the University of Toronto Scarborough and co-author of <a href="https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2023-90247-001?doi=1">a new study published in the journal <em>Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts</em></a>.</p> <p>“Those babies were dealing with all these distractions in the concert hall, but still had these uninterrupted bursts of attention.”</p> <p>The findings suggest that even babies feel the impact of being at a live show, through both musicians’ interactions with an audience and the social experience of being in a crowd.&nbsp;Cirelli recalls moments during the performance when a calm would sweep over the babies, and other times when a change in pitch or vocal riff would excite them all.</p> <p>She says this may offer insights into why humans are hardwired to consume music and attend live shows.</p> <p>“If there’s something happening that we collectively are engaging with, we’re also connecting with each other. It speaks to the shared experience,” says Cirelli, director of <a href="https://www.utsc.utoronto.ca/labs/cirelli/">the TEMPO Lab</a>, which studies how infants and children respond to music.</p> <p>“The implication is that this is not necessarily specific to this one performance. If there’s these moments that capture us, then we are being captured together.”</p> <p>It’s well established that socialization is crucial during early childhood development&nbsp;– an infant’s brain is laying the groundwork for future life skills and abilities as it grows. Cirelli says music can play a powerful part in making those important bonds. She points to research finding <a href="/news/babies-prefer-familiar-tune-even-if-it-s-sung-stranger-u-t-study">infants are more likely to socialize with someone after hearing them sing a familiar song</a> or dancing to music with them, and that infants have strong emotional reactions to music and song even before their first birthday.</p> <p>“We consistently find that music can be a highly social and emotional context within which infants can foster connections to their caregivers, other family members and even new acquaintances,” she says. “This audience study shows that even in a community context, infants are engaging with the music and connecting to their fellow audience members.”</p> <figure role="group" class="caption caption-drupal-media align-center"> <div> <div class="field field--name-field-media-image field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="lazy" src="/sites/default/files/styles/scale_image_750_width_/public/2023-07/SMR_DSC04448%5B60%5D-crop.jpg?itok=IG5cSsBZ" width="750" height="500" alt="&quot;&quot;" class="image-style-scale-image-750-width-"> </div> </div> <figcaption><em>Babies watched a selection of songs from </em>The Music Box<em>, an operatic performance designed for infants<br> (submitted photo)</em></figcaption> </figure> <p>For the study, researchers examined 120 babies ages six to 14 months as they watched a children’s opera performed at a concert hall that doubles as a research facility at McMaster University (61 babies watched in person, while the other 59 watched a recorded version).</p> <p>Researchers meticulously broadcast the recording so that the performers were at the same size, distance and volume as the live version. The babies’ responses were tracked through heart monitors and tablets mounted on the backs of concert seats. Later, student research assistants combed through the footage to note when babies looked at the stage and when they looked away.</p> <p>The live performance captured the babies' attention for 72 per cent of the 12-minute show while the recording held their attention for 54 per cent of the time. The live show also had infants continuously watching for longer bouts of time.&nbsp;</p> <p>“Even little babies who may or may not have experienced music in a community context before are already engaging more when it’s delivered this way,” Cirelli says.</p> <p>“That’s one question we have as music cognition researchers: What is it about the live experience that's worth it? Why would people go if there’s not something fundamental about that live music experience that's above and beyond listening to music by yourself?”</p> <p>That’s not to say babies find virtual performances boring. After the onset of the pandemic, the researchers virtually studied one group of babies as they watched the same recording in their homes over Zoom. Those babies paid about as much attention as the ones who attended the live show – watching about 64 per cent on average – but they were more likely to become &nbsp;distracted and have shorter bursts of attention.&nbsp;</p> <p>“The babies watching at home didn’t have the distraction of being in a new place&nbsp;– they were in their comfort zone,” Cirelli says. "But even without distractions, the quality of their attention was still not nearly as strong as the audience in the live condition.”</p> <p>The study&nbsp;– which was co-authored by former TEMPO Lab postdoctoral researcher&nbsp;<strong>Haley Kragness</strong>,<strong>&nbsp;</strong>now an assistant professor at Bucknell University&nbsp;– will also feed into some of Cirelli’s other work.</p> <p>In a different study, she and a team of researchers are exploring whether a live performance over Zoom has the same impact on engagement as a live performance in person, and whether musicians’ interactions with an audience can play a similarly powerful role in capturing attention.</p> <p>Yet another study will investigate whether live performances affect their memory of the event and how watching a live performance versus a recorded version affects how they feel about the performer.</p> <p>“If a baby is frequently brought to these kinds of events, will that shape their foundation for engaging in music and the community later in childhood?” Cirelli asks.</p> <p>“It speaks to why we even engage with music at all.”</p> <p>The study&nbsp;was funded by the 鶹Ƶ <a href="https://connaught.research.utoronto.ca/opportunities">Connaught New Researcher Award</a> and by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC).</p> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-news-home-page-banner field--type-boolean field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">News home page banner</div> <div class="field__item">Off</div> </div> Mon, 17 Jul 2023 19:22:00 +0000 lanthierj 302284 at Researchers explore shifting AI landscape at Absolutely Interdisciplinary conference /news/researchers-explore-shifting-ai-landscape-absolutely-interdisciplinary-conference <span class="field field--name-title field--type-string field--label-hidden">Researchers explore shifting AI landscape at Absolutely Interdisciplinary conference</span> <div class="field field--name-field-featured-picture field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="eager" srcset="/sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_370/public/2023-07/June21_JamieNapier_DSC01162-crop.jpg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=A3lGwdWu 370w, /sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_740/public/2023-07/June21_JamieNapier_DSC01162-crop.jpg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=qZV6LhSf 740w, /sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_1110/public/2023-07/June21_JamieNapier_DSC01162-crop.jpg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=IkjT6MVq 1110w" sizes="(min-width:1200px) 1110px, (max-width: 1199px) 80vw, (max-width: 767px) 90vw, (max-width: 575px) 95vw" width="740" height="494" src="/sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_370/public/2023-07/June21_JamieNapier_DSC01162-crop.jpg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=A3lGwdWu" alt="&quot;&quot;"> </div> <span class="field field--name-uid field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden"><span>siddiq22</span></span> <span class="field field--name-created field--type-created field--label-hidden"><time datetime="2023-07-12T15:00:25-04:00" title="Wednesday, July 12, 2023 - 15:00" class="datetime">Wed, 07/12/2023 - 15:00</time> </span> <div class="clearfix text-formatted field field--name-field-cutline-long field--type-text-long field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Cutline</div> <div class="field__item"><p><em>Participants at the Schwartz Reisman Institute for Technology and Society’s annual conference, Absolutely Interdisciplinary, discussed what AI can teach us about social systems, cognition, education, creativity and more (supplied image)</em></p> </div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-topic field--type-entity-reference field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Topic</div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/topics/our-community" hreflang="en">Our Community</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-story-tags field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden field__items"> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/schwartz-reisman-institute-technology-and-society" hreflang="en">Schwartz Reisman Institute for Technology and Society</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/artificial-intelligence" hreflang="en">Artificial Intelligence</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/centre-ethics" hreflang="en">Centre for Ethics</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/computer-science" hreflang="en">Computer Science</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/machine-learning" hreflang="en">machine learning</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/ontario-institute-studies-education" hreflang="en">Ontario Institute for Studies in Education</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/psychology" hreflang="en">Psychology</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/research-innovation" hreflang="en">Research &amp; Innovation</a></div> </div> <div class="clearfix text-formatted field field--name-body field--type-text-with-summary field--label-hidden field__item"><p>Researchers across disciplines gathered recently at the&nbsp;<a href="https://absolutelyinterdisciplinary.com/">Absolutely Interdisciplinary</a> conference to reckon with the ramifications of the evolving AI landscape – from the technology’s transformative impacts on work and education, to its implications for human cognition and values.</p> <p>The third annual academic conference held by the&nbsp;<a href="https://srinstitute.utoronto.ca/">Schwartz Reisman Institute for Technology and Society</a> (SRI) at the University of Toronto featured 23 speakers from diverse fields such as computer science, psychology, law, economics, education, philosophy, media studies, and literature.</p> <p>“The recent impacts of generative AI tools have really emphasized how important it is to build spaces for interdisciplinary conversations,” said SRI Director and Chair&nbsp;<a href="https://www.law.utoronto.ca/faculty-staff/full-time-faculty/gillian-hadfield"><strong>Gillian Hadfield</strong></a>, the inaugural&nbsp;Schwartz Reisman Chair in Technology and Society, CIFAR AI Chair and professor in the Faculty of Law and Rotman School of Management.</p> <p>“Our goal with Absolutely Interdisciplinary is to foster new research agendas based in the creative interplay of diverse questions and framings as we explore the potentials of these new technologies.”&nbsp;</p> <h4>Cognition, storytelling and the future of intelligence</h4> <p>The conference featured an opening keynote by <a href="https://absolutelyinterdisciplinary.com/speakers-2023#Aguera-y-Arcas">Blaise Agüera y Arcas</a>, vice-president and fellow at Google Research, and a regular participant in developing cross-disciplinary dialogues about AI and ethics, fairness and bias, policy and risk.</p> <p>“We’re all going through a bit of existential angst because most of us believe that we have been the smartest things on Earth for some time now,” he said. “I don’t think that will any longer be the case.”</p> <figure role="group" class="caption caption-drupal-media align-center"> <div> <div class="field field--name-field-media-image field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="lazy" src="/sites/default/files/styles/scale_image_750_width_/public/2023-07/June21_JamieNapier_DSC00822.jpg?itok=NjJVEONy" width="750" height="500" alt="&quot;&quot;" class="image-style-scale-image-750-width-"> </div> </div> <figcaption><em>In his keynote lecture, </em><strong><em>Blaise Agüera y Arcas</em></strong><em> of Google Research spoke about the history and potential future of AI development (supplied image)</em></figcaption> </figure> <p>Exploring the historical connections between neuroscience, computer science and cognitive science, Agüera y Arcas discussed the progress made in visual perception by neural networks in recent years, and delved into the concept of artificial general intelligence (AGI), including <a href="https://srinstitute.utoronto.ca/news/risk-and-uncertainty-what-should-we-do-about-ai">ongoing debates</a> on whether AI might become sentient and pose a risk to humans.</p> <p>“Some people take AGI to mean superintelligence, others take it to mean consciousness,” said Agüera y Arcas, who highlighted storytelling as an essential quality of human experience, and how the construction of personal narratives about ourselves and our past help to predict our future.</p> <p>“We are the stories that we tell ourselves,” Agüera y Arca noted. “And as we interact with people, we construct and edit that story over time.”</p> <h4>AI, learning and the role of education</h4> <p>In a session pairing psychology with computational neuroscience, Joel Leibo of Google DeepMind and SRI Faculty Fellow <a href="https://www.psych.utoronto.ca/people/directories/all-faculty/william-cunningham"><strong>William Cunningham</strong></a>, a professor in 鶹Ƶ’s department of psychology, presented their ongoing work on modeling human social interactions using artificial agents to test <a href="https://socialcognitivescience.ca/william-cunningham/">social cognitive theory</a>.&nbsp;</p> <p>Leibo and Cunningham showed that by simulating coordination games in multi-agent reinforcement learning, they could test the origins of very human things like in-group bias and coalition-building.</p> <p>SRI Research Lead <a href="https://www.cs.toronto.edu/~ashton/"><strong>Ashton Anderson</strong></a>, assistant professor in the department of computer science, moderated another session on learning from a different perspective: the implications of generative AI tools such as ChatGPT for educators and students.</p> <p>Panelist <a href="https://www.jewishstudies.utoronto.ca/people/directories/all-faculty/lauren-bialystok"><strong>Lauren Bialystok</strong></a>, an associate professor of social justice education at the Ontario Institute for Studies in Education and acting director of 鶹Ƶ’s <a href="https://ethics.utoronto.ca/">Centre for Ethics</a>, offered ideas on why we value the concept of “originality” and discipline students who violate it by using ChatGPT to write assignments.</p> <p>“What is the benchmark against which cheating emerges as a moral wrong or a pedagogical error?” asked Bialystok.</p> <p>“Is technology the enemy of originality? What about individual originality versus collective originality? We need to start sussing out what really matters to us in student learning and student assessment.”</p> <figure role="group" class="caption caption-drupal-media align-center"> <div> <div class="field field--name-field-media-image field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="lazy" src="/sites/default/files/styles/scale_image_750_width_/public/2023-07/June21_JamieNapier_DSC01029.jpg?itok=9CEUFVaJ" width="750" height="500" alt="&quot;&quot;" class="image-style-scale-image-750-width-"> </div> </div> <figcaption><em>From left to right: Moderator Nicolas Papernot with panelists William Cunningham and Joel Leibo, who described how multi-agent reinforcement learning can be used to test aspects of social cognitive theory (supplied image)</em></figcaption> </figure> <h4>AI’s capabilities, behaviours and harms</h4> <p>One of the more frequent fears cited about AI is that it will replace workers&nbsp;– a subject tackled in a session moderated by SRI Research Lead <a href="https://www.avigoldfarb.com/"><strong>Avi Goldfarb</strong></a>, the Rotman Chair in Artificial Intelligence and Healthcare and a professor of marketing at the Rotman School of Management. E<span style="font-size: 1rem;">conomist <a href="https://absolutelyinterdisciplinary.com/speakers-2023#rock">Daniel Rock</a> of the University of Pennsylvania’s Wharton School shared his <a href="https://arxiv.org/abs/2303.10130">recent findings</a> on which occupations are most likely to be impacted by large language models.</span></p> <p>“People who process information [and] have knowledge as part of their work are more exposed,” said Rock, who noted that this exposure can be harmful or helpful.</p> <p>“One of the key things for economists to add to this conversation is equilibrium,” Rock said. “It’s not just about AI replacing workers&nbsp;– there’s complementary innovation to be done here; there’s supply and demand; there’s the question of whether making one part of work cheap makes another part of it very expensive. There is a lot more work to be done here.”</p> <h4>New languages, new frameworks for understanding</h4> <p>The conference closed with a session on AI and creativity led by SRI Faculty Fellow <a href="https://www.english.utoronto.ca/people/directories/all-faculty/avery-slater"><strong>Avery Slater</strong></a>, an assistant professor in the department of English in the Faculty of Arts &amp; Science. The session featured presentations by literary scholar N. Katherine Hayles, whose work has focused on relations between science, literature and technology, and U.K.-based poet Polly Denny, who presented her experiments with text-generating AI systems that have yielded new forms of artistic collaboration.</p> <p>Where Agüera y Arcas’s opening keynote&nbsp;highlighted the role of narration about the self as constitutive of what we might think of as AGI, Hayles also framed the question of whether AI is “alive” in similarly fundamental terms of how perception and meaning-making construct and define the environments in which we operate.</p> <p>“Where there's life, there’s cognition. But computational media also have cognitive capabilities,” Hayles said.</p> <h3><a href="https://srinstitute.utoronto.ca/news/absolutely-interdisciplinary-2023-ignites-new-conversations-and-insights-on-ai-research">Read the full story at the Schwartz Reisman Institute</a></h3> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-news-home-page-banner field--type-boolean field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">News home page banner</div> <div class="field__item">Off</div> </div> Wed, 12 Jul 2023 19:00:25 +0000 siddiq22 302231 at Children are learning even if they don't pay attention: Study /news/children-are-learning-even-if-they-don-t-pay-attention-study <span class="field field--name-title field--type-string field--label-hidden">Children are learning even if they don't pay attention: Study</span> <div class="field field--name-field-featured-picture field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="eager" srcset="/sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_370/public/2023-06/GettyImages-1240140094-crop.jpg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=BbjFaJDd 370w, /sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_740/public/2023-06/GettyImages-1240140094-crop.jpg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=FUkunMYN 740w, /sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_1110/public/2023-06/GettyImages-1240140094-crop.jpg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=rdsD1_LV 1110w" sizes="(min-width:1200px) 1110px, (max-width: 1199px) 80vw, (max-width: 767px) 90vw, (max-width: 575px) 95vw" width="740" height="494" src="/sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_370/public/2023-06/GettyImages-1240140094-crop.jpg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=BbjFaJDd" alt="a father and son look at a laptop screen together while the son does is homework"> </div> <span class="field field--name-uid field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden"><span>Christopher.Sorensen</span></span> <span class="field field--name-created field--type-created field--label-hidden"><time datetime="2023-06-21T14:57:25-04:00" title="Wednesday, June 21, 2023 - 14:57" class="datetime">Wed, 06/21/2023 - 14:57</time> </span> <div class="clearfix text-formatted field field--name-field-cutline-long field--type-text-long field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Cutline</div> <div class="field__item"><p><em>(photo by&nbsp;Marko Geber/Getty Images)</em></p> </div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-author-reporters field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden field__items"> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/authors-reporters/chris-sasaki" hreflang="en">Chris Sasaki</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-story-tags field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden field__items"> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/alumni" hreflang="en">鶹Ƶ</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/brain" hreflang="en">Brain</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/children" hreflang="en">Children</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/faculty-arts-science" hreflang="en">Faculty of Arts &amp; Science</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/psychology" hreflang="en">Psychology</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/research-innovation" hreflang="en">Research &amp; Innovation</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-subheadline field--type-string-long field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Subheadline</div> <div class="field__item">'Children's brains can hold information in a way that adults' brains do not'</div> </div> <div class="clearfix text-formatted field field--name-body field--type-text-with-summary field--label-hidden field__item"><p>A team of researchers has shown that children’s apparent inability to pay attention allows them to outperform adults when it comes to retaining information they were instructed to disregard.</p> <p>The researchers’ study shows that, as expected, adults do a great job of focusing their attention on an assigned task and do not pay attention to information they are told to ignore.</p> <p>Children, on the other hand, take&nbsp;in the secondary information they are instructed to ignore when given the same task. The information is then encoded in their brains.</p> <p>“What we found is that children's brains can hold information in a way that adults' brains do not,” says <strong>Yaelan Jung</strong>, who worked on the study as a graduate student at the University of Toronto and in her current position as a postdoctoral researcher at Emory University.</p> <figure role="group" class="caption caption-drupal-media align-left"> <div> <div class="field field--name-field-media-image field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="lazy" src="/sites/default/files/2023-06/graphic-icons-lead.jpg" width="370" height="270" alt="illustrations of a palm tree, bee, couch and car"> </div> </div> <figcaption><em>Researchers used a series of four simple icons to test how well adults and children paid attention (image courtesy of Jung, Finn, et al.)</em></figcaption> </figure> <p>“Although it’s not a foreign idea that children have poorer attention abilities than adults, we did not know how this poor attention would impact the way their brains receive and hold other information,” she says. “Our study fills this knowledge gap and shows that children's poor attention leads them to hold more information from the world than adults.”</p> <p>The team described their study <a href="https://www.jneurosci.org/content/43/21/3849">in a paper published in the<em>&nbsp;Journal of Neuroscience</em></a>.</p> <p>In addition to Jung, the authors include:&nbsp;<strong>Tess Forest</strong>,&nbsp;who also contributed to the study as a graduate student at 鶹Ƶ and in her current position as a postdoctoral research scientist at Columbia University; and&nbsp;<strong>Dirk Bernhardt-Walther</strong>&nbsp;and&nbsp;<strong>Amy Finn&nbsp;</strong>–&nbsp;both associate professors in the Faculty of Arts &amp; Science’s&nbsp;department of psychology.</p> <p>“It’s not simply that children’s ability to pay attention is bad and they’re unable to disregard distractors,” says Finn. “Our study suggests that their brains are built to be sensitive to all information, regardless of whether it's relevant or not – that kids are more sensitive to more information.</p> <p>“Depending on your definition of childhood, humans are children for eight or nine years,” she says. “Compared to other species, that’s a long time and one explanation for such a lengthy childhood is that we humans have so much learning to do. Another is that it’s important for our IQ to take in as much information as we do. Still another is that we need to take in all this information as children in order to wire our brains properly, to develop the circuits and pathways for processing information.”</p> <p>The study involved 24 adults with a mean age of 23 years and 26 children with a mean age of eight years. The team asked the participants to observe a series of four static illustrations: a bumble bee, a car, a chair and a tree. Each image was accompanied by a background of grey dots moving in one of four directions: up, down, left and right.</p> <p>In one phase of the study, subjects were instructed to ignore the moving dots and press a button when an object – say, the bumblebee – appeared more than once. In another phase, they were asked to ignore the objects and press a button when the direction of motion of the dots was repeated.</p> <p>Subjects carried out their task while in a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) machine at the <a href="https://toni.psych.utoronto.ca/">Toronto Neuroimaging Facility</a> at the University of Toronto. As they performed the task, the MRI measured the subjects’ brain activity which revealed how attention shapes what is represented in subjects’ brains.</p> <p>“What we found in this study provides a novel way to think about what brain development means,” says Jung. “Often, we assume that as the brain develops it will do more and do things better. Thus, we often think that adults are better and smarter than kids. However, our work shows this is not always the case. Rather, children's brains may just do things differently than adults – and consequently, they can sometimes do more than adults.”</p> <p>Added Finn: “The study suggests that this approach of being more sensitive to the broader environment, at the cost of paying attention to specific things, is better for understanding complex systems. It may help form a higher level of understanding of our full environment.</p> <p>“So, I look at kids as these little information-processing creatures better able to represent more of the world, with brains that more accurately reflect the world than ours.”</p> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-news-home-page-banner field--type-boolean field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">News home page banner</div> <div class="field__item">Off</div> </div> Wed, 21 Jun 2023 18:57:25 +0000 Christopher.Sorensen 302053 at Researchers explore how we depict and perceive emotions through colour and line in visual art /news/researchers-explore-how-we-depict-and-perceive-emotions-through-colour-and-line-visual-art <span class="field field--name-title field--type-string field--label-hidden">Researchers explore how we depict and perceive emotions through colour and line in visual art</span> <div class="field field--name-field-featured-picture field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="eager" srcset="/sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_370/public/2023-05/joy-one-color.jpeg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=XgYYm_Dv 370w, /sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_740/public/2023-05/joy-one-color.jpeg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=NIUXVyz7 740w, /sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_1110/public/2023-05/joy-one-color.jpeg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=WaI25POG 1110w" sizes="(min-width:1200px) 1110px, (max-width: 1199px) 80vw, (max-width: 767px) 90vw, (max-width: 575px) 95vw" width="740" height="494" src="/sites/default/files/styles/news_banner_370/public/2023-05/joy-one-color.jpeg?h=afdc3185&amp;itok=XgYYm_Dv" alt="Reference illustration from the 鶹Ƶ study, including yellow, green and orange colours"> </div> <span class="field field--name-uid field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden"><span>siddiq22</span></span> <span class="field field--name-created field--type-created field--label-hidden"><time datetime="2023-05-03T15:41:52-04:00" title="Wednesday, May 3, 2023 - 15:41" class="datetime">Wed, 05/03/2023 - 15:41</time> </span> <div class="clearfix text-formatted field field--name-field-cutline-long field--type-text-long field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Cutline</div> <div class="field__item"><p>A new study by 鶹Ƶ researchers confirms the use of certain colours and lines to depict emotions such as joy, as shown in this reference illustration from the study (Damiano, Bernhardt-Walther, et al.)</p> </div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-author-reporters field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden field__items"> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/authors-reporters/chris-sasaki" hreflang="en">Chris Sasaki</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-topic field--type-entity-reference field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">Topic</div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/topics/breaking-research" hreflang="en">Breaking Research</a></div> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-story-tags field--type-entity-reference field--label-hidden field__items"> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/faculty-staff" hreflang="en">Faculty &amp; Staff</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/graduate-students" hreflang="en">Graduate Students</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/psychology" hreflang="en">Psychology</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/research" hreflang="en">Research</a></div> <div class="field__item"><a href="/news/tags/visual-arts" hreflang="en">Visual Arts</a></div> </div> <div class="clearfix text-formatted field field--name-body field--type-text-with-summary field--label-hidden field__item"><p>Are you feeling blue –&nbsp;or seeing red? Maybe turning green with envy?</p> <p>You’re not alone in colour-coding your emotions, University of Toronto researchers say in a new paper confirming associations between feelings and certain shades and shapes.</p> <figure role="group" class="caption caption-drupal-media align-left"> <div> <div class="field field--name-field-media-image field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="lazy" src="/sites/default/files/2023-05/DirkBWalther_small.png" width="250" height="348" alt="Dirk Bernhardt-Walther"> </div> </div> <figcaption><em>Dirk Bernhardt-Walther</em></figcaption> </figure> <p>In a <a href="http://jov.arvojournals.org/Article.aspx?articleid=2785495">new study in the <em>Journal of Vision</em></a>, researchers from the Faculty of Arts &amp; Science's <a href="https://www.psych.utoronto.ca/">department of psychology</a>&nbsp;and their collaborators have confirmed research identifying consistent associations between certain colours and lines, and particular emotions.</p> <p>In addition, they’ve shown that it is easier to predict the emotion being depicted with colour drawings than line drawings; and that emotion predictions are more accurate for colour drawings by non-artists than by artists.</p> <p>“What we confirmed in our study was the systematic use of certain colours and lines to depict certain emotions,” says&nbsp;<strong><a href="https://www.psych.utoronto.ca/people/directories/all-faculty/dirk-bernhardt-walther">Dirk Bernhardt-Walther</a></strong>,&nbsp;an associate professor in the department of psychology.</p> <p>“For example, anger is depicted using red, or in drawings with densely packed lines. Sadness is blue and associated with vertical lines. We use these conventions to portray emotions –&nbsp;and observers perceive the emotions intended.”</p> <p>The findings could help designers and visual artists convey emotions to users or viewers, or create architectural or designed spaces that evoke positive responses. It could also lead to a better understanding of visual esthetics –&nbsp;how artists depict emotions in their work and whether it evokes the response they desire from viewers.</p> <p>The study's lead author is&nbsp;<a href="https://www.claudiadamiano.ca/home">Claudia Damiano</a>, a postdoctoral researcher with the <a href="https://ppw.kuleuven.be/bc">department of brain and cognition</a>&nbsp;at KU Leuven in&nbsp;Belgium, and a former graduate student in Bernhardt-Walther's lab. Damiano&nbsp;conducted the research&nbsp;with <a href="https://scholar.google.com/citations?user=HQxKjvIAAAAJ&amp;hl=en">Pinaki Gayen</a>, a visiting graduate student who came to 鶹Ƶ's department of psychology in 2019 on a Shastri Indo-Canadian Institute Research Fellowship. 鶹Ƶ co-authors include Bernhardt-Walther and postdoctoral fellow <a href="https://mrezanejad.github.io/">Morteza Rezanejad</a>, also&nbsp;in the department of psychology.</p> <p>For the study, Bernhardt-Walther and his colleagues recruited 40 students from visual arts programs at OCAD University and 41 non-artists from STEM programs at 鶹Ƶ. All were instructed to create two abstract drawings – one using colour and one lines –&nbsp;for each of six emotions: anger, disgust, fear, sadness, joy and wonder.</p> <p>The researchers began by validating the idea that distinct emotions were depicted in a consistent manner. First, they conducted computational analysis of the lines and colours in all the drawings. They then built a computational model that could predict the emotion from the visual properties of drawings by artists and non-artists.</p> <p>They found that drawings depicting negative emotions tended to contain more lines and darker colours: red, blue, brown, black and grey. Drawings of positive emotions were less dense, had more curved or oblique lines&nbsp;and contained brighter colours.</p> <p>Images for joy were predominantly yellow-green, those depicting disgust were&nbsp;a darker green,&nbsp;anger was shown as&nbsp;red while sadness was blue,&nbsp;and so on. The line drawings exhibited different styles of lines –&nbsp;from strong, intersecting lines for anger, to wavy and curved lines for joy.</p> <figure role="group" class="caption caption-drupal-media align-right"> <div> <div class="field field--name-field-media-image field--type-image field--label-hidden field__item"> <img loading="lazy" src="/sites/default/files/2023-05/i1534-7362-23-4-1-f1_1680251416-crop.jpeg" width="350" height="377" alt="Sample colour and line drawings for each emotion"> </div> </div> <figcaption><em>Sample colour and line drawings for each emotion, made by one artist and one non-artist participating in the study (Damiano, Bernhardt-Walther, et al.)</em></figcaption> </figure> <p>The team also compared how artists and non-artists conveyed emotions with colours and found that trained artists generally used a smaller number of colours than non-artists and that the colours they used were unconventional. They also discovered that non-artists were better at conveying emotions through colour than artists.</p> <p>“I believe the reason for this difference could be that non-artists tend to follow convention, whereas artists strive to be innovative&nbsp;–&nbsp;they want to do something distinctive,” Bernhardt-Walther says. “Artists know what the conventions are&nbsp;but they want to break from those conventions in order to provoke, stand out and create something special.”</p> <p>The researchers also found that it is easier to guess the emotion a colour drawing is portraying than in&nbsp;a line drawing. They speculate that this is because the associations between colours and emotions are stronger for people&nbsp;than those&nbsp;between lines and emotions.</p> <p>And while the study did not delve into whether these associations are innate or learned, Bernhardt-Walther draws on his own research and that of other academics, noting these colour-emotion matches aren’t just culturally learned –&nbsp;in other words, we didn’t learn them simply from the paintings, illustrations and movies viewed throughout our lives.</p> <p>“There is generally very good agreement on the association between colours and emotions across cultures that have developed independently,” Bernhardt-Walther says.</p> <p>“There is consensus that red has significance because it is associated with blood&nbsp;–&nbsp;whether it’s your prey’s blood or your own. Our faces turn red when we are angry and grey or green when we feel nauseous. Darkness is scary because of the unknown danger.</p> <p>“And in addition to being associated with sadness, blue is also calming –&nbsp;and the obvious association with the sky and water and&nbsp;<a href="https://www.artsci.utoronto.ca/news/u-t-researchers-provide-insight-how-we-sense-threats-our-environment">being in the open where you are less at risk</a>&nbsp;from a danger like a predator. We imitate these colours in artwork to specifically evoke these emotions.”</p> <p>For Bernhardt-Walther, the study is consistent with his growing interest in the effect of the visual environment on our emotions.</p> <p>“I’m studying visual esthetics more and more now as part of my research,” he says.</p> <p>“I want to know what people find esthetically pleasing and why, because I think it is an integral part of our perceptual experience. Liking or disliking what we see is directly related to how we think and how we perceive the world.”</p> </div> <div class="field field--name-field-news-home-page-banner field--type-boolean field--label-above"> <div class="field__label">News home page banner</div> <div class="field__item">Off</div> </div> Wed, 03 May 2023 19:41:52 +0000 siddiq22 301483 at